Skip to Content

17 Insect Species That Behave Like Mammals

Red Flat Bark Beetles: Arctic Adaptation
Red Flat Bark Beetles: Arctic Adaptation (image credits: pixabay)

While insects and mammals evolved along different evolutionary paths, certain insect species have independently developed behaviors that strikingly resemble those of mammals. These convergent behaviors—from parental care to social structures and even farming—highlight how similar solutions can evolve to address common challenges across vastly different taxonomic groups. This fascinating phenomenon showcases nature’s remarkable ability to develop comparable behavioral strategies despite the enormous genetic and physiological differences between insects and mammals.

Social Structures in Termites Rivaling Mammalian Societies

Termites
Termites. Image via Depositphotos.

Termites have evolved social systems that bear remarkable similarities to those found in mammalian societies. Species like Macrotermes natalensis construct enormous mounds that function as self-regulating environments with sophisticated temperature and humidity control—a feat comparable to how beavers engineer their surroundings. Within these structures, termites operate with a division of labor resembling mammalian social groups, with distinct castes including workers, soldiers, and reproductive individuals. Their colonies can contain millions of individuals organized in hierarchical systems that parallel the complex social arrangements seen in many mammal species. Perhaps most remarkably, certain termite species practice monogamy among their reproductive pairs, a trait relatively uncommon in insects but prevalent among mammals that form pair bonds for reproductive purposes.

Parental Care in Burying Beetles

black beetle on brown rock in close up photography during daytime
Dung beetle. Image via Unsplash

Nicrophorus or burying beetles demonstrate extraordinary parental investment that mirrors mammalian parenting behaviors. These beetles locate small dead vertebrates, bury them to create a “crypt,” and then prepare the carcass by removing hair or feathers and shaping it into a ball. The female lays eggs nearby, and after hatching, both parents feed their larvae regurgitated carrion—a behavior strikingly similar to mammalian nursing. Studies have shown that parent beetles respond to begging behaviors from their young, adjusting food provision based on offspring signals, much as mammalian parents respond to their young’s hunger cues. Additionally, burying beetles protect their brood from predators and competitors, demonstrating a level of parental investment rarely seen outside mammals and birds but critical to offspring survival in both groups.

Elaborate Nest Construction by Leaf-Cutter Ants

Leaf-Cutter Ants Image via Pixels

Leaf-cutter ants of the genera Atta and Acromyrmex construct some of the most complex nests in the insect world, rivaling the den-building behaviors of mammals like badgers or prairie dogs. Their underground colonies feature specialized chambers serving distinct purposes—nurseries for young, fungus gardens for food production, and waste disposal areas—similar to how mammals organize their living spaces. The most sophisticated leaf-cutter ant colonies contain up to 8 million individuals and extend 30 meters horizontally and 6 meters deep, with carefully regulated temperature and humidity gradients. Like burrowing mammals that engineer their environment, these ants create complex ventilation systems that maintain optimal conditions for their fungus gardens. Their ability to modify their environment to such a degree represents a level of niche construction more commonly associated with mammals than with insects.

Agricultural Practices of Fungus-Growing Termites

Ant
Matabele Ants, Gorongosa National Park, Mozambique. These ants raid termite nests. Here they are shown carrying their prey back to their own nest. Judy Gallagher, CC BY 2.0, via Wikimedia Commons

Macrotermitinae termites have evolved agricultural practices that parallel human farming behaviors. These termites cultivate Termitomyces fungi in specialized chambers within their mounds, creating controlled environments with precise temperature and humidity regulation. Like mammalian farmers, they actively manage their fungal crops—adding new plant material as substrate, weeding out competing fungi, and harvesting their crop for consumption. Research has revealed that these termites use antimicrobial compounds in their saliva to selectively suppress competing fungi while promoting their cultivated species. This agricultural relationship has evolved over approximately 30 million years, creating a mutual dependency where neither partner can survive without the other—similar to the co-evolution between humans and their domesticated crops. The parallel evolution of agriculture in these insects and in human societies represents one of the most striking examples of convergent behavior across distant taxonomic groups.

Cooperative Hunting in Army Ants

black ant, insect, garden ant, lasius niger, animalia, arthropod, ants, ants, ants, ants, ants, ants
Army ants. Photo by SandeepHanda via Unsplash.

Army ants, particularly species in the genera Eciton and Dorylus, exhibit coordinated hunting behaviors that strongly resemble pack hunting in mammalian predators like wolves or lions. These ants form massive raiding parties—sometimes containing over a million individuals—that sweep through forest habitats in coordinated formations. Communication between individuals during hunts occurs through pheromone trails and direct contact, allowing them to encircle prey and overcome creatures many times their size. Like mammalian pack hunters, army ants employ specialized roles during hunts: larger soldier ants subduing prey while smaller workers dismantle and transport the captured food. This division of labor improves hunting efficiency, just as different roles enhance success in mammalian hunting groups. The evolutionary pressure to obtain protein in competitive environments has independently driven both these insect species and mammalian predators to develop remarkably similar cooperative hunting strategies.

Milk Production in Cockroaches

How to get rid of cockroaches overnight
Cockroach. Image by Erik Kartis via Pexels.

The Pacific beetle cockroach (Diploptera punctata) displays a form of “milk” production that represents one of the most striking mammalian-like behaviors in the insect world. Unlike egg-laying insects, this species gives birth to live young and produces a milk-like substance containing proteins, fats, and sugars to nourish its developing embryos. This insect milk is secreted from the mother’s brood sac and contains protein crystals that are among the most nutrient-dense substances known in the animal kingdom—three times richer in calories than buffalo milk. The embryonic cockroaches digest this milk using specialized gut structures, similar to how mammalian offspring digest their mother’s milk. This convergent evolution of maternal nutrition delivery represents a fascinating example of how similar physiological solutions can develop independently across distant evolutionary branches to solve the challenge of offspring nourishment.

Sophisticated Communication in Honey Bees

bee, insect, pollinate, pollination, flower, winged insect, wings, nature, hymenoptera, entomology, macro, honey bee, nectar, flower background, beautiful flowers, close up, animal, flower wallpaper, world bee day
bee, insect, pollinate, pollination, flower, winged insect, wings, nature, hymenoptera, entomology, macro, honey bee, nectar, flower background, beautiful flowers, close up, animal, flower wallpaper, world bee day. Image via Unsplash

Honey bees (Apis mellifera) have evolved communication systems with complexity rivaling those of many mammals. Their famous waggle dance represents one of the most sophisticated symbolic communication systems outside humans—a form of abstract representation where the orientation and duration of the dance convey precise information about distant food sources. Like mammalian communication, honey bee signals incorporate multiple sensory channels, including visual, tactile, and chemical components. Bees also demonstrate social learning, where inexperienced individuals acquire knowledge from experienced foragers, similar to cultural transmission in mammalian societies. Recent research has even revealed that honey bees can communicate abstract concepts like “above” or “below,” demonstrating cognitive abilities once thought unique to vertebrates. The parallel evolution of complex communication in honey bees and mammals highlights how similar selective pressures for efficient resource utilization can drive the development of sophisticated information-sharing systems across distant evolutionary lineages.

Thermoregulation in Bumble Bees

Bumblebees live in colonies, not hives.
Bumblebees live in colonies, not hives. Image by Davies Designs via Unsplash.

Bumble bees (Bombus species) demonstrate remarkable endothermic abilities typically associated with mammals rather than insects. Unlike most insects, which depend on external heat sources, bumble bees can regulate their body temperature independently of ambient conditions through metabolic heat generation. By uncoupling their wing muscles from their wings and vibrating them rapidly—a process called shivering thermogenesis—bumble bees can maintain thoracic temperatures around 35°C even when environmental temperatures drop below 10°C. This ability allows them to forage in cold conditions that would incapacitate most insects. Even more impressively, bumble bee colonies collectively regulate nest temperatures to support brood development, with worker bees incubating developing larvae by pressing their warm abdomens against brood cells—a behavior strikingly similar to how mammals incubate their young. Some species even form “heat chains” where workers transfer warmth from the nest interior to peripheral brood chambers, demonstrating a level of cooperative thermoregulation usually associated with social mammals.

Sleep Patterns in Fruit Flies

Moths
Fruit Flies Image via Depositphotos.

Drosophila melanogaster, the common fruit fly, exhibits sleep patterns with surprising similarities to mammalian sleep. Research has demonstrated that these flies experience distinct sleep states characterized by increased arousal thresholds, reduced responsiveness to stimuli, and specific body postures—all hallmarks of mammalian sleep. Like mammals, fruit flies that are sleep-deprived show subsequent rebound sleep, indicating homeostatic regulation similar to mammalian sleep cycles. Studies using electroencephalogram-like recordings have even identified different sleep phases in fruit flies that parallel the REM and non-REM sleep states found in mammals. Perhaps most fascinating is the discovery that fruit flies possess neural circuits regulating sleep that use similar neurotransmitters (including dopamine and GABA) to those controlling mammalian sleep—despite the vast evolutionary distance between these groups. This remarkable convergence suggests that sleep serves fundamental biological functions that have driven similar adaptive solutions across diverse evolutionary lineages.

Tool Use in Ants

Trap-jaw ant
Trap-jaw ant. Image by Openverse.

Certain ant species demonstrate sophisticated tool use behaviors that parallel those seen in mammals like primates and elephants. Aphaenogaster ants collect small objects like sand grains, leaf fragments, and twigs to use as “sponges” for transporting liquid food back to their nests—a behavior requiring evaluation of tool suitability and planning. Even more remarkably, some species actively modify materials to create more effective tools; for instance, Atta cephalotes workers tear pieces from leaves specifically sized for transporting liquid. Black imported fire ants (Solenopsis richteri) use soil particles to extract liquid food from surfaces that are otherwise difficult to collect from, adjusting the size of particles based on the viscosity of the liquid—demonstrating tool selection based on task requirements. Like tool use in mammals, these behaviors are often socially transmitted through colonies and represent complex cognitive adaptations for environmental manipulation that were once thought exclusive to vertebrates with larger brains.

Teaching Behaviors in Tandem-Running Ants

Sahara Desert Ant running along the sand
Ant running along the sand. Image by kingmaphotos@gmail.com via Depositphotos.

Temnothorax albipennis ants engage in a remarkable form of teaching behavior during tandem running—a process whereby knowledgeable ants lead naive nestmates to resources. This behavior meets all scientific criteria for teaching: the instructor modifies its behavior in the presence of a naive individual, incurs a cost (tandem runs take longer than individual journeys), and the naive individual acquires knowledge more rapidly than it would independently. During these teaching sessions, the leader ant periodically pauses to allow the follower to survey surroundings and memorize landmarks, adjusting pace based on feedback taps from the learning ant—a bidirectional communication system reminiscent of how mammalian parents teach offspring. If the follower loses contact, the leader searches for it rather than continuing alone, demonstrating commitment to the teaching process. This sophisticated knowledge transfer mechanism parallels teaching behaviors in mammalian species, highlighting how selective pressures for efficient information sharing can produce similar cognitive adaptations across distant taxonomic groups.

Defensive Herding in Aphids

green aphid
green aphid. Photo by Viktor Forgacs, via Unsplash

Certain aphid species display collective defensive behaviors that strikingly resemble the herding responses seen in ungulate mammals threatened by predators. When attacked by predatory insects like ladybugs or lacewings, colonies of pea aphids (Acyrthosiphon pisum) exhibit coordinated responses where individuals release alarm pheromones that trigger group dispersal behavior. More remarkably, some aphid species, including the gall-forming Pemphigus spyrothecae, respond to threats by forming tight clusters with juveniles protected in the center—a formation analogous to the defensive circles formed by musk oxen or wildebeest. Some soldier aphids even sacrifice themselves to protect their colonies, similar to defensive behaviors in certain mammal species. These collective defensive strategies benefit aphid colonies by confusing predators, diluting individual risk, and protecting vulnerable members—the same evolutionary advantages that drove the development of herding behaviors in prey mammals facing predation pressure.

Warfare and Territory Defense in Weaver Ants

a spider crawling on a green leaf
Weaver Ant. Image via Unsplash

Oecophylla weaver ants engage in territorial conflicts that parallel warfare in mammalian species like chimpanzees. These ants maintain distinct territories spanning multiple trees, with clearly defined boundaries that are actively patrolled and defended against neighboring colonies. When territories overlap, scouts from rival colonies engage in ritualized combat that can escalate to full colony warfare involving thousands of individuals. Like mammalian territorial conflicts, these battles follow distinct phases, including initial assessment, escalation, and either resolution or full-scale combat. Weaver ants use sophisticated tactics during these conflicts, including coordinated ambushes and strategic positioning of defenders at vulnerable points. Most remarkably, successful colonies will progressively claim territory from defeated neighbors, expanding their range through conquest—a behavior strikingly similar to territorial expansion in social mammals. The evolution of such complex territorial behaviors in both insects and mammals reflects similar selective pressures for resource defense across divergent evolutionary lineages.

Problem-Solving in Bumblebees

Bumblebees' wings beat 200 times per second.
Bumblebees’ wings beat 200 times per second. Image by Krzysztof Niewolny via Unsplash.

Bumblebees demonstrate cognitive abilities in problem-solving that were once considered exclusive to mammals. Research has shown that these insects can learn to pull strings to access rewards—a task requiring both spatial reasoning and understanding of cause-effect relationships. Even more impressively, naive bumblebees can learn this skill by observing experienced individuals, demonstrating social learning capabilities similar to those found in mammals. Studies at Queen Mary University of London revealed that bumblebees can even innovate solutions to novel problems; when presented with a puzzle box containing sugar water, they learned to rotate balls into specific positions to access the reward—a task no bee would encounter in nature. Like intelligent mammals, bumblebees show behavioral flexibility, adapting their approaches when faced with changing challenges. Their remarkable cognitive abilities emerge from brains containing fewer than a million neurons (compared to a mouse’s 70 million), challenging assumptions about the neural requirements for complex cognition and highlighting how similar selection pressures can produce comparable cognitive adaptations across distant evolutionary lineages.

Trap Construction by Antlion Larvae

Firefly larvae.
Firefly larvae. Source: YouTube. Uploaded: Brave Wilderness.

Antlion larvae (Myrmeleontidae family) construct elaborate pit traps that parallel the hunting strategies of trap-building mammals like the fennec fox. These larvae excavate conical pits in sandy soil, precisely engineered with unstable slopes that cause prey to tumble toward the predator waiting at the bottom. Like mammalian trap builders, antlions select optimal construction sites based on soil composition, sun exposure, and prey traffic patterns. They actively maintain their traps by removing debris and repairing damage—demonstrating ongoing investment similar to how mammalian predators maintain their hunting grounds. When prey falls into the pit, antlions employ sophisticated capture techniques, including flicking sand to trigger miniature landslides that bring the prey within reach. Some species even adjust pit dimensions based on their hunger levels and prey availability, showing flexibility in hunting strategy reminiscent of mammalian predators. This convergent evolution of trap construction between insects and mammals illustrates how similar hunting challenges can drive the development of comparable predatory adaptations despite vast phylogenetic differences.

Navigation Abilities in Dung Beetles

Upis ceramboides. Image via Openverse

Dung beetles, particularly nocturnal species like Scarabaeus satyrus, demonstrate navigation abilities comparable to mammals that use celestial cues for orientation. These beetles collect dung into balls and need to move them away from the competitive dung pile in straight lines to avoid returning and facing competition. To maintain straight-line travel, they use the Milky Way as a celestial compass—the only insects known to navigate by the galaxy rather than individual stars or the moon. Like navigating mammals, dung beetles create mental maps of their surroundings and can retain these spatial memories over time. Laboratory experiments have shown that dung beetles wearing tiny hats that block their view of the sky move in confused circles, while those with clear views maintain straight paths. This sophisticated celestial navigation system parallels how some mammals use stars for orientation during migrations, representing a remarkable case of convergent evolution in spatial cognition between vastly different taxonomic groups.

Farming Behavior in Ambrosia Beetles

S. Rae from Scotland, UK, CC BY 2.0 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0, via Wikimedia Commons

Ambrosia beetles (subfamilies Scolytinae and Platypodinae) practice sophisticated fungal farming behaviors that parallel agricultural practices in humans. These beetles bore into trees and cultivate fungal gardens that convert wood—indigestible to the beetles—into nutritious food. Female beetles carry fungal spores in specialized structures called mycangia, effectively “planting” their crop in new galleries. Like human farmers, they actively maintain their fungal gardens by removing competing fungi, aerating the cultures, and controlling moisture levels. Some species even use their fecal secretions as fertilizers to promote fungal growth. Remarkably, different ambrosia beetle species cultivate distinct fungal strains specialized for their particular needs—similar to how human societies have developed specific crop varieties. This agricultural mutualism has evolved independently in at least seven beetle lineages, representing one of the most sophisticated.

Conclusion:

black june beetle on gray stone close-up photography
black june beetle on gray stone close-up photography. Image via Unsplash

The remarkable parallels between insect and mammalian behaviors reveal the powerful role of convergent evolution in shaping life across diverse branches of the tree of life. Despite vast differences in size, brain structure, and evolutionary history, certain insect species have independently developed behaviors once thought unique to mammals—from parental care and communication to farming, tool use, and even teaching. These striking similarities underscore how common ecological challenges—such as raising offspring, finding food, or defending territory—can lead to the emergence of analogous solutions in organisms that are worlds apart. Insects, often overlooked in discussions of complex behavior, emerge from this comparison as surprisingly sophisticated problem-solvers, architects, and caregivers. Their behavioral innovations challenge our assumptions about intelligence, social organization, and adaptation, reminding us that nature often finds similar answers to life’s questions—no matter the species.